A Systemic view of the learning and differentiation of scientific concepts:The case of electric current and voltage revisited

Main Article Content

Ismo T Koponen
Tommi Kokkonen

Abstract

In learning conceptual knowledge in physics, a common problem is the incompleteness of a learning process, where students’ personal, often undifferentiated concepts take on more scientific and differentiated form. With regard to such concept learning and differentiation, this study proposes a systemic view in which concepts are considered as complex, dynamically evolving structures. The dynamics of the concept learning and differentiation is driven by the competition of model utility in explaining the evidence. Based on the systemic view, we introduce computational model, which represents the essential features of the conceptual system in the form of directed graph (DGM), where concepts are nodes connected to other conceptual elements (nodes) in the graph. The results of a DGM are then compared to the empirical findings to identify differentiation between concepts of electric current and voltage based on a re-analysis of previously published empirical findings on upped secondary school students’ learning paths in the context of DC circuits. The comparison shows that the model predicts and explains many relevant, empirically observed features of the learning paths of concept learning and differentiation, such as: 1) Context-dependent dynamics, 2) the persistence of ontological shift and concept differentiation, and 3) the effects of communication on individual learning paths.  The systemic view and the DGM model based on it make these generic features of interest in concept learning and differentiation understandable and show that these features are associated with the guidance of theoretical knowledge. Finally, we discuss briefly the implications of the results on teaching and instruction. 


Article Details

How to Cite
Koponen, I. T., & Kokkonen, T. (2014). A Systemic view of the learning and differentiation of scientific concepts:The case of electric current and voltage revisited. Frontline Learning Research, 2(3), 140–166. https://doi.org/10.14786/flr.v2i2.120
Section
Articles

References

Andersen, H. Barker, B. and Chen, X. (2006). The Cognitive Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Andersen, H. and Nersessian, N. J. (2000). Nomic Concepts, Frames, and Conceptual Change. Philosophy of Science, 67, S224-S241.
Brown, D. E., & Hammer, D. (2008). Conceptual Change in Physics. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change (pp. 127–154). New York: Routledge.
Carey, S. (2010). The Origin of Concepts. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Chi, M. T. H., & Slotta, J. D. (1993). The Ontological Coherence of Intuitive Physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 249-260.
Chi, M. T. H. (2005). Commonsense Conceptions of Emergent Processes: Why Some Misconceptions Are Robust. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14, 161-199. DOI: 10.1207/s15327809jls1402_1.
Chi, M. T. H. (2008). Three Types of Conceptual Change: Belief Revision, Mental Model Transformation, and Categorical Shift. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change (pp. 35–60). New York, NY: Routledge.
Chi, M. T. H., & Brem, S. K. (2009). Contrasting Ohlsson's Resubsumption Theory With Chi's Categorical Shift Theory'. Educational Psychologist, 44, 58 — 63. DOI: 10.1080/00461520802616283.
Cohen, R., Eylon, B., & Ganiel, U. (1983). Potential Difference and Current in Simple Electric Circuits: A Study of Students’ Concepts. American Journal of Physics, 51, 407-412.
Danks, D. (2010). Not different kinds, just special cases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 208-209.DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X1000052X
Engelhardt, P. V., & Beichner, R. J. (2004). Students’ Understanding of Direct Current Resistive Electrical Circuits. American Journal of Physics, 72, 98-115. DOI: 10.1119/1.1614813.
Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Words, Thoughts, and Theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gupta, A., Hammer, D., & Redish, E. F. (2010). The Case for Dynamic Models of Learners’ Ontologies in Physics. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 19, 285-321. DOI: 10.1080/10508406.2011.537977.
Henderson, L., Goodman, N. D., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Woodward, J. F. (2010). The Structure and Dynamics of Scientific Theories: A Hierarchical Bayesian Perspective. Philosophy of Science, 77, 172–200.
Hoyningen-Huene, P. (1993). Reconstructing Scientific Revolutions: Thomas S. Kuhn’s Philosophy of Science. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
Jeong, H & Chi, M. T. H. (2007). Knowledge convergence and collaborative learning. Instructional Science, 35, 287–315. DOI: 10.1007/s11251-006-9008-z.
Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, Kinds and Conceptual Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Koponen, I. T. (2013). Systemic View of Learning Scientific Concepts: A Description in Terms of Directed Graph Model. Complexity, 19, 27-37. DOI: 10.1002/cplx.21474.
Koponen I. T. and Huttunen L. (2013). Concept Development in Learning Physics: The Case of Electric Current and Voltage. Science & Education, 22, 2227-2254. DOI: 10.1007/s11191-012-9508-y.
Koumaras, P., Kariotoglou, P. & Psillos, D. (1997). Causal Structures and Counter-intuitive Experiments in Electricity. International Journal of Science Education, 19, 617–630.
Lee, Y., & Law, N. (2001). Explorations in Promoting Conceptual Change in Electrical Concepts via Ontological Category Shift. International Journal of Science Education, 23, 111- 149.
Machery, E. (2009). Doing without Concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Murphy, G. L. (2004). The Big Book of Concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Nersessian, N. (2008) Creating Scientific Concepts. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.
Ohlsson, S. (2009). Resubsumption: A Possible Mechanism for Conceptual Change and Belief Revision. Educational Psychologist, 44, 20-40. DOI: 10.1080/00461520802616267.
Ohlsson, S. (2011). Deep Learning: How the Mind Overrides Experience. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Rehder, B. (2003). Categorization as Causal Reasoning. Cognitive Science, 27, 709–748.
Reiner, M., Slotta, J. D., Chi, M. T. H., & Resnick, L. B. (2000). Naive Physics Reasoning: A Commitment to Substance Based Reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 18, 1-34.
Shipstone, D. M. (1984). A Study of Children’ s Understanding of Electricity in Simple DC
Circuits. European Journal of Science Education, 6, 185- 198.
Slotta, J. D., & Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Helping Students Understand Challenging Topics in Science Through Ontology Training. Cognition and Instruction, 24, 261-289.
Smith, C., Carey, S., & Wiser, M. (1985). On differentiation: A Case Study of the Development of the Concept of Size, Weight and Density. Cognition, 21, 177-237.
Smith, E. E., & Medin, D. L. (1981). Categories and Concepts. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.
Weinberger, A., Stegmann, K., Fischer, F. (2007) Knowledge convergence in collaborative learning: Concepts and assessments. Learning and Instruction, 17, 416-426.